Low costs of allonursing in meerkats: mitigation by behavioural change?

Is cooperation really cooperation if it imposes no cost on the giver? Two things are usually assumed about cooperative behaviour: that it provides a benefit to the intended receiver, such as the removal of ectoparasites from groomed individuals, and that it incurs some cost to the individual participating: whether that’s time that could be spent doing other things (such as eating), or an energetic cost (in terms of energy expended).

In my most recent paper, published in Behavioral Ecology, I wanted to find out whether this was true of one particular “cooperative” activity, allonursing (the nursing of offspring that aren’t yours) in meerkats. Allonursing occurs regularly in meerkat groups, and has been being recorded as part of the Kalahari Meerkat Project long-term database since 1996, giving us a great opportunity to look at this behaviour in some details, with lots of data. The meerkat system is also useful for asking these questions because of their quirky breeding biology. Here, instead of having a few females which hunker down together in a communal nest and nurse each others’ young (meaning that all are donors and recipients of allonursing, in the sense that their offspring are receiving it), in meerkats, only one female breeds (she/her offspring are recipients), while other females help her to rear her offspring without necessarily having any of their own (they are donors of allonursing). This makes the costs and benefits easier to figure out – because here they aren’t net costs/benefits as they would be in communal systems…

… like this!

Capture
In communally breeding species (a) mothers are simultaneously donors and recipients of allonursing, meaning that while mothers and offspring may benefit from offspring receiving allonursing, quantification of these benefits is likely to be confounded by costs (energetic costs to mothers of providing milk to non-offspring, costs to offspring of maternal resources being diverted to non-siblings). In singular cooperative breeders (b) only one female in the social group produces offspring, which are raised by subordinate helpers (denoted “SUB”) not concurrently rearing young of their own, so help is not reciprocal. Resultantly, the costs of providing allonursing are not confounded by the benefits of receiving it and vice versa. Dashed lines enclose individuals likely to incur costs through allonursing; dotted lines enclose individuals likely to benefit from allonursing. Small circles denote offspring.

I was also interested to find out if females alter their behaviour to mitigate the costs of allonursing (if it did in fact turn out to carry a cost). Imagine you were going to run 10km every day for three weeks. To help you meet the energetic costs of doing that much high-octane exercise for such a long period you might do two things: eat more, to make up for the energy you’d lose on your runs; and you might also dial back on other activities to help conserve energy – skipping your pilates class, for example, or getting an extra hour’s rest at night. This sort of behavioural modification is seen in lactating animal mothers: lactation is thought to be such an energetic slog that mothers benefit from considerably upping their food intake (as in saddleback and cotton-top tamarins: Goldizen 1987; Price 1992), or spending less time doing other activities, such as in baboons, which spend more time resting when lactating (Barrett et al. 2002).

baboon
not lazy… just “conserving energy”

In order to achieve these two aims, I looked at two things: how allonursing influenced weight change, using body mass data from the long term database; and how allonurses differed from non-allonursing females (of the same age and weight range) in their behaviour during the period of lactation.

Body mass is an important predictor of success in meerkats, which is why I decided to use this measure to test costs of allonursing. Heavier females are more likely to survive, breed, and attain dominance  (Clutton-Brock et al. 2001; Russell et al. 2003; Hodge et al. 2008), so if contributing to allonursing results in weight loss, this could spell problems for females hoping to eventually have young of their own. However, although lactation is thought to be a hugely costly aspect of offspring care (Clutton-Brock et al. 1989), allonursing female meerkats didn’t lose more weight overnight (when allonursing occurs) than non-allonursing females, and didn’t lose a significant amount of weight over the whole allonursing period (though they don’t gain weight, as non-allonursing females did over the same time). The latter suggests that there might be some cost of allonursing, as these females aren’t able to pile on the weight as their non-nursing counterparts do – but as they don’t actually lose weight the long term costs for survival are likely to be low.

(A separate analysis – that didn’t end up being part of the paper due to low sample size – showed the same patterns when female contributions to cooperative behaviours were taken into account: investment in cooperative behaviours such as raised guarding, burrow maintenance, and babysitting didn’t influence weight change over the lactation period, but whether they allonursed or not did.)

To determine whether allonursing females changed their behaviour to mitigate the costs of allonursing, I headed to the field to gather some behavioural data to supplement existing focal observation data (following individual meerkat females for 30 minutes at a time and recording their every move). From this data I took four behaviours of particular interest: raised guarding, and burrow maintenance (cooperative behaviours that I expected allonurses to do less of when lactating), and active foraging and resting (which I expected allonurses to do more of when lactating).

4
The Kalahari desert – not a terrible place to work, even if it is just following meerkats around all day… From Van Zylsrus Travel Information: http://www.south-africa-info.co.za/country/town/553/van-zylsrus

Only one of these behaviours fulfilled my predictions: allonursing females did forage more when they were lactating (relative to pre- and post-lactation measures, and relative to non-allonursing females). Contrary to expectations, though, there was no difference in how much time they spent resting, and instead of decreasing time spent participating in other costly cooperative behaviours, allonurses increased their input in raised guarding and burrow maintenance while lactating.

Saddleback-tamarin-portrait
Like saddleback tamarin mothers, do meerkat allonurses increase rate of foraging while lactating?

So what does this actually tell us? It seems that the jury is still out on whether allonursing actually carries a cost to subordinate meerkat females. Given the substantial costs of lactation to mammalian mothers, what are we missing? In this paper I propose two possible answers. First, allonursing may be a “low-cost” behaviour, for example, because most allonurses have recently lost litters of their own, meaning they may have excess milk (MacLeod et al. 2013). Allonursing only when the “costs” are low occurs in other species – such as lions, for example, which are tolerant of sneaky cubs trying to suckle only when they have small litters and can afford an extra occasional mouth to feed, or when their own litters are older and less reliant on their milk (Pusey & Packer 1994). Alternatively, the extra foraging that allonurses appear to do may successfully mitigate the costs of allonursing so that those costs are not apparent – despite the unexpected increase, not decrease, in cooperative behaviours.

Of course there are issues with this type of approach, which prevent us from making definitive conclusions, and which necessitates the need for further work. It wasn’t possible to manipulate allonursing status in such a way that allows us to control for other factors that may be associated with allonursing (ie. make some females lactate, and prevent others from doing so) – as it is, the allonursing group in this study are self-selected, and though we controlled for as much as possible in our models, it is of course always possible that we’re missing something that’s linked to that self-selection.

Hopefully this paper suggests some interesting avenues of further research. That I didn’t find any really obvious costs of allonursing in meerkats, though it could indicate that they successfully mitigate costs, makes it important that we try to quantify how much milk is actually being transferred. This will allow us to compare the relative costs and contributions of allonursing to maternal lactation more meaningfully.

References

Barrett L, Halliday J, Henzi SP. 2006. The ecology of motherhood:the structuring of lactation costs by chacma baboons. J Anim Ecol. 75:875–886
Clutton-Brock TH, Albon SD, Guinness FE. 1989. Fitness costs of gestation and lactation in wild mammals. Nature. 337:260–262.
Clutton-Brock TH et al. 2001.Cooperation, control, and concession in meerkat groups. Science. 291:478–481.
Goldizen AW. 1987. Facultative polyandry and the role of infant-carrying in wild saddle-back tamarins (Saguinus fuscicollis). Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 20:99–109
Hodge SJ et al. 2008. Determinants of reproductive success in dominant female meerkats. J Anim Ecol. 77:92–102.
MacLeod KJ, Nielsen JF, Clutton-Brock TH. 2013. Factors predicting the frequency, likelihood and duration of allonursing in the cooperatively breeding meerkat. Anim Behav. 86:1059–1067
Price E. 1992. Changes in the activity of captive cotton-top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus) over the breeding cycle. Primates. 33:99–106
Pusey AE, Packer C. 1994. Non-offspring nursing in social carnivores: minimizing the costs. Behav Ecol. 5:362–374
Russell AF et al. 2003a. Breeding success in cooperative meerkats: effects of helper number and maternal state. Behav Ecol. 14:486–492

Porpoise Killing Seals…Dramatic New Behaviour!

Fascinating seal behaviour – who knew??

Deep Blue Conversations

Scientists have discovered through DNA analysis that harbour porpoises (Phocoena phocoena) that have washed up severely mutilated and dead along the Dutch as well as the Belgian and northern France coastline beaches with an unknown cause of death and mutilation were actually attacked by grey seals (Halichoerus grypus). This is a new behaviour for the normally fish eating mammals.

Grey Seal. Image via www.theanimalfiles.com Grey Seal. Image via http://www.theanimalfiles.com

Two different papers came out this past month (Leopold et al and Jauniaux et al) confirming this behaviour in the North Sea though DNA analysis (as well as a Note in Marine Ecology Progress Series on the ability to extract usable DNA from bite wounds). By collecting swab samples from bite marks on washed up harbour porpoises scientist were able to confirm that the injuries and death had been inflicted by Grey Seals.  Along the Dutch coastline 721 of 1,081 stranded porpoises were examined by Leopold et al between…

View original post 773 more words